Lời khen năng lực và lời khen nỗ lực đối với sự tự tin vào năng lực của sinh viên

Nhiều nghiên cứu đã khám phá ảnh hưởng khác biệt của lời khen năng lực

và lời khen nỗ lực đến sự tự tin vào năng lực của học sinh nhỏ nhưng còn

hạn chế trên nhóm sinh viên. Đây là một sự thiếu hụt quan trọng bởi việc

khen ngợi và mức độ tự tin vào năng lực có liên quan mật thiết đến quần thể

sinh viên, và sinh viên tuổi trưởng thành so với nhóm học sinh nhỏ tuổi có

sự khác biệt quan trọng trong cách nhìn nhận lời khen năng lực và lời khen

nỗ lực. Hơn nữa, khác biệt văn hóa trong quan điểm về lời khen của người

Việt và người nước ngoài cũng có thể cản trở việc khái quát hóa các phát

hiện trước đó. Vì thế, nghiên cứu này muốn tìm hiểu liệu khen ngợi về năng

lực và nỗ lực có sự liên hệ khác nhau đến mức độ tự tin vào năng lực của giới

trẻ Việt Nam, và liệu mức độ chân thành của lời khen có điều tiết mối quan

hệ này. Tổng cộng 403 sinh viên đại học được khảo sát trực tuyến về mức độ

tự tin vào năng lực tổng quát và năng lực học thuật, tần suất được khen về

năng lực và nỗ lực, và mức độ chân thành của những lời khen đó. Phân tích

mô hình tuyến tính cho thấy, lời khen năng lực là một yếu tố dự báo mạnh

hơn cho sự tự tin vào năng lực. Ngoài ra, mức độ chân thành chỉ điều tiết

đáng kể tần suất lời khen năng lực nhưng không điều tiết tần suất lời khen

nỗ lực. Những kết quả này sau đó được thảo luận về cách các nhà giáo dục

và cha mẹ nên áp dụng sự khen ngợi như một hình thức thuyết phục xã hội

để nâng cao mức độ tự tin vào năng lực của sinh viên.

pdf21 trang | Chia sẻ: Thục Anh | Ngày: 18/05/2022 | Lượt xem: 249 | Lượt tải: 0download
Bạn đang xem trước 20 trang nội dung tài liệu Lời khen năng lực và lời khen nỗ lực đối với sự tự tin vào năng lực của sinh viên, để xem tài liệu hoàn chỉnh bạn click vào nút DOWNLOAD ở trên
little above the recipient’s ability. Other researchers have also agreed on the importance of sincerity in praising (Bandura, 1999; Delin & Baumeister, 1994). However, we found that sincerity did not moderate the effect of frequent effort praise, which contradicts Henderlong and Lepper (2002) who suggest that the effects of both ability and effort praise should be moderated by sincerity. Henderlong and Lepper (2002) argue that if effort praise does 588 not match with one’s level of expended effort, i.e. praising a person for their hard work when they think that they did not expend much effort, the praise will be disregarded (Henderlong & Lepper, 2002). Here, we argue that self-judgment of one’s effort is not as simple as self-judgment of one’s ability, because effort is a more complex construct for assessment. It might be possible that one person expends a great deal of effort but perceives that it is not high enough, while another person who does not put as much effort as required for the difficulty level of a task but self-judges that their effort is substantial. In other words, judging effort is like judging a whole process of trying, which is invisible, intangible, and hard to quantify (e.g. nights of sleeplessness, personal struggle, and time spent on task), while judging ability is like judging the end product, which is visible, tangible and quantifiable (e.g. a final test score). Thus, because there is no clear benchmark to judge effort, evaluations of effort can be easily influenced by other people’s evaluations because the evaluators themselves (both the recipient of praise who evaluates their own effort and the giver of praise who evaluates other people’s effort) are unsure and unconfident of how correctly they evaluate the expended effort. If this is the case, then we argue that both sides (the recipient and the giver) will find an equilibrium to match their effort evaluations, resulting in praise that always seems sincere. We found at least two experiments that investigated the effect of reward on the judgement of effort. In 2015, Pooresmaeili et al. asked 26 adult participants to perform a task requiring physical effort by continuously pressing two keyboards to push a ball up a virtual ramp. Half of the participants in the experimental group received an amount of money equivalent to the level of difficulty they reached, and the other half in the control group also received a monetary reward but was random and unrelated to the task difficulty. Their results showed that participants revised their self-judgement of effort after receiving the rewards, suggesting that reward magnitude has an effect on self-judgement of effort. When receiving higher rewards, participants overestimated their effort, with a converse effect observed for lower rewards. Later, Rollwage et al. (2020) replicated and extended this research design to investigate whether this reward magnitude effect still exists for evaluators who judge other people’s efforts. Using the same experimental paradigm, 51 participants were asked 589 to rate both their own and other participants’ efforts on the same ball task. The results were parallel with Pooresmaeili et al. (2015), showing that higher rewards were associated with higher effort rated for both self– and others-judgement. As praise can be a kind of verbal reward, based on these studies, we suggest that being praised for high effort by other people makes one reassesses their own level of effort, whether or not they truly expended much effort. Thus, frequent effort praise might always work even if it is exaggerated or understated. Several limitations in this study will be discussed with suggestions for future direction. Firstly, because of the retrospective cross-sectional design, our result still implies a bidirectional relationship between praise and self-efficacy. While much of our discussion is on how praise can be a booster of self-efficacy, it is still possible that highly self-efficacious people tend to receive more praises because they achieve more than low self-efficacious peers, hence the report of a positive association between frequency of praise and self-efficacy. In order to make causal conclusion on the effect of praise, more experiments should be done in the future, in which participants should be assigned into at least three groups: receiving ability praise, receiving effort praise, and receiving no praise. Secondly, future research can explore qualitatively and quantitatively how Vietnamese perceive ability praise and effort praise differently from a cultural standpoint. In real-life circumstances, praising is oftentimes a form of greetings in Vietnamese culture (Ngô Hương Lan, 2016), which can make it more susceptible to be non-specific and untrustworthy. This cultural characteristic of Vietnamese people should be considered when designing studies on praise. Thirdly, the majority of our sample were students from one university in Ho Chi Minh city and were mostly females, which may hinder the generalization of our results. Fourthly, because we did not use back-translation when translating the GSE and CASES scales, misunderstandings might have occurred that hinder accurate measurements. Future studies should always use back-translation when using non-Vietnamese scales. Finally, it would be necessary to explore whether our findings on praise and self-efficacy are also applied to other special and underrepresented groups such as Vietnamese students with learning disabilities (LD). As it has been found that students with LD often 590 underestimate their self-efficacy (Seyed et al., 2017) and social persuasion is not a significant source of efficacy information (Hampton, 1998), using praise to persuade that they are capable might not work in the same way as for typical functioning students. Thus, more research is warranted to examine how ability and effort praise work differently for Vietnamese students with LD. V. CONCLUSION Our findings support Bandura’s self-efficacy theory that social persuasion in forms of verbal praise is a significant predictor of self- efficacy. Here, we found that different types of praise, including ability and effort praise, have differential and sophisticated associations with self- efficacy in general and in academic contexts. Furthermore, regardless of which type of praise is given, sincerity of praise, i.e. the extent to which the recipient of praise perceives that the praise matches themselves, is an important factor. As the first study looking at the role of praise on self- efficacy of Vietnamese university students, we hope that our results give a few highlights to inform the practice of praising for teachers and parents, that if ability praise is used, it needs to be frequent and honest, while effort praise can be either frequent or honest. REFERENCES In Vietnamese Bùi Thị Phương Chi & Phạm Thị Thu Hà (2018). Một vài khảo sát về đặc điểm văn hóa của người Châu Âu và người Việt thể hiện qua lời khen. Retrieved from: mot-vai-khao-sat-ve-dac-diem-van-hoa-cua-nguoi-chau-au-va-nguoi- viet-the-hien-qua-loi-khen Ngô Hương Lan (2016). Đặc trưng văn hóa ứng xử của người Việt Nam và người Nhật Bản qua một số hành vi. Đề tài cấp Viện thuộc Viện Nghiên cứu Đông Bắc Á. Retrieved from: Nguyễn Văn Quang (1999). Một số khác biệt giao tiếp lời nói Việt – Mỹ trong cách thức khen và tiếp nhận lời khen. Luận án Tiến Sĩ, Đại học Quốc gia Hà Nội. Retrieved from: luanan?a=d&d=TTkGWOSplxFa1999&e=-------vi-20--1--img- txIN-------# 591 In English Amemiya, J., & Wang, M. T. (2018). Why effort praise can backfire in adolescence. Child Development Perspectives, 12(3), 199-203. https://doi.org/10.1111/ cdep.12284 Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychological Review, 84(2), 191-215. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033- 295X.84.2.191 Bandura, A. (1982). Self-efficacy mechanism in human agency. American psychologist, 37(2), 122. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.37.2.122 Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. W. H. Freeman and Company. Bhanji, J. P., & Delgado, M. R. (2014). The social brain and reward: Social information processing in the human striatum. WIREs Cognitive Science, 5(1), 61-73. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1266 Delin, C. R., & Baumeister, R. F. (1994). Praise: More than just social reinforcement. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour, 24(3), 219-241. https://doi. org/10.1111/j.1468-5914.1994.tb00254.x Hampton, N. Z. (1998). Sources of academic self-efficacy scale: An assessment tool for rehabilitation counselors. Rehabilitation Counseling Bulletin, 41(4), 260–277. Henderlong, J., & Lepper, M. R. (2002). The effects of praise on children’s intrinsic motivation: A review and synthesis. Psychological Bulletin, 128(5), 774- 795. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.128.5.774 Holloway, S. D. (1988). Concepts of ability and effort in Japan and the United States. Review of Educational Research, 58(3), 327-345. https://doi. org/10.3102/00346543058003327 Ifdil, I., Bariyyah, K., Dewi, A. K., & Rangka, I. B. (2019). The college academic self-efficacy scale (CASES): An Indonesian validation to measure the self- efficacy of students. Jurnal Kajian Bimbingan dan Konseling, 4(4), 115-121. Jain, S., Bruce, M. A., Stellern, J., & Srivastava, N. (2007). Self-efficacy as a function of attributional feedback. Journal of School Counseling, 5(4), n4. Muenks, K., & Miele, D. B. (2017). Students’ thinking about effort and ability: the role of developmental, contextual, and individual difference factors. Review of Educational Research, 87(4), 707-735. https://doi. org/10.3102/0034654316689328 592 Nguyễn Thị Xuân Hồng & Phan Thị Tuyết Nga (2020). Students’ self-efficacy beliefs and TOEIC achievements in the Vietnamese context. International Journal of Instruction, 13(4), 67-86. https://doi.org/10.29333/iji.2020.1345a Nicholls, J. G. (1976). Effort is virtuous, but it’s better to have ability: Evaluative responses to perceptions of effort and ability. Journal of Research in Personality, 10(3), 306-315. https://doi.org/10.1016/0092-6566(76)90020-9 Nicholls, J. G. (1978). The Development of the concepts of effort and ability, Perception of academic attainment, and the understanding that difficult tasks require more ability. Child Development, 49(3), 800. https://doi. org/10.2307/1128250 Owen, S. V., & Froman, R. D. (1988). Development of a college academic self- efficacy scale. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the National Council on Measurement in Education. New Orleans, LA. Pooresmaeili, A., Wannig, A., & Dolan, R. J. (2015). Receipt of reward leads to altered estimation of effort. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112(43), 13407-13410. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1507527112 Rollwage, M., Pannach, F., Stinson, C., Toelch, U., Kagan, I., & Pooresmaeili, A. (2020). Judgments of effort exerted by others are influenced by received rewards. Scientific Reports, 10(1). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020- 58686-0 Schunk, D. H. (1982). Effects of effort attributional feedback on children’s perceived self-efficacy and achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 74, 548-556. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.74.4.548 Schunk, D. H. (1983a). Ability versus effort attributional feedback: Differential effects on self-efficacy and achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 75(6), 848-856. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.75.6.848 Schunk, D. H. (1983b). Reward contingencies and the development of children’s skills and self-efficacy. Journal of Educational Psychology, 75(4), 511. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.75.4.511 Schunk, D. H. (1984). Enhancing self-efficacy and achievement through rewards and goals: Motivational and informational effects. The Journal of Educational Research, 78(1), 29-34. https://doi.org/10.1080/00220671.198 4.10885568 Schunk, D. H. (1991). Self-efficacy and academic motivation. Educational psychologist, 26(3-4), 207-231. https://doi.org/10.1080/00461520.1991.96 53133 593 Schwarzer, R., & Jerusalem, M. (1995). Generalized self-efficacy scale. In J. Weinman, S. Wright, & M. Johnston (Eds.), Measures in health psychology: A user’s portfolio. Causal and control beliefs, 35-37. Windsor, UK: NFER- NELSON. Seyed, S., Salmani, M., Motahari Nezhad, F., & Noruzi, R. (2017). Self-efficacy, achievement motivation, and academic progress of students with learning disabilities: A comparison with typical students. Middle East Journal of Rehabilitation and Health, 4(2). https://doi.org/10.5812/mejrh.44558 The jamovi project (2021). jamovi. (Version 1.8) [Computer Software]. Retrieved from https://www.jamovi.org. Tzur, S. K., Ganzach, Y., & Pazy, A. (2016). On the positive and negative effects of self-efficacy on performance: Reward as a moderator. Human Performance, 29(5), 362-377. https://doi.org/10.1080/08959285.2016.1192631 Truong, T. N. N., & Wang, C. (2019). Understanding Vietnamese college students’ self-efficacy beliefs in learning English as a foreign language. System, 84, 123-132. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.system.2019.06.007 Van Dinther, M., Dochy, F., & Segers, M. (2011). Factors affecting students’ self- efficacy in higher education. Educational Research Review, 6(2), 95-108. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.edurev.2010.10.003 Woulfe, J. (2008). Self-efficacy and culture: A comparison of Denmark and the United States. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 64(1), 17-29. Xing, S., Gao, X., Jiang, Y., Archer, M., & Liu, X. (2018b). Effects of ability and effort praise on children’s failure attribution, self-handicapping, and performance. Frontiers in Psychology, 9. https://doi.org/10.3389/ fpsyg.2018.01883 Zuffianò, A., Alessandri, G., Gerbino, M., Luengo Kanacri, B. P., di Giunta, L., Milioni, M., & Caprara, G. V. (2013). Academic achievement: The unique contribution of self-efficacy beliefs in self-regulated learning beyond intelligence, personality traits, and self-esteem. Learning and Individual Differences, 23, 158-162. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2012.07.010

Các file đính kèm theo tài liệu này:

  • pdfloi_khen_nang_luc_va_loi_khen_no_luc_doi_voi_su_tu_tin_vao_n.pdf
Tài liệu liên quan